How Do Brain Networks Differentiate Between New and Old Information?
A study has found that the cortex acts like a "memory machine", encoding new experiences and predicting the near future.

Complete the form below to unlock access to ALL audio articles.
The cerebral cortex is the largest part of a mammal’s brain, and by some measures the most important. In humans in particular, it’s where most things happen—like perception, thinking, memory storage, and decision-making. One current hypothesis suggests that the cortex’s primary role is to predict what’s going to happen in the future by identifying and encoding new information it receives from the outside world and comparing it with what was expected to occur.
A new study published today in the journal Neuron takes a big step toward proving that hypothesis. The paper’s lead author is Yuriy Shymkiv, a postdoctoral fellow in the lab of Professor Rafael Yuste.
“We found that the cortex acts like a memory machine, encoding new experiences, and predicting the very near future,” Shymkiv said.
“This study gives a great deal of insight into the role of the cerebral cortex, and into diseases like schizophrenia where the cortex seems to be malfunctioning,” Yuste said, noting that it also helps clarify important processes in the normal brain. “Novelty is the difference between what you predicted will happen and what actually occurred. This research shows that the cerebral cortex is continuously detecting novel stimuli, in order to change and improve its predictions of the future. Novelty detection is a critical function for humans and other animals.”
Want more breaking news?
Subscribe to Technology Networks’ daily newsletter, delivering breaking science news straight to your inbox every day.
Subscribe for FREETo deepen their understanding of these findings, the team built a neural network model of the auditory cortex and trained it to detect stimuli that are new. It replicated what they had seen in mice, showing that networks of neurons also used activity “echoes” to store a model of the environment, and used it to detect change. They concluded that the way the cortex is wired, with loops of connected neurons, makes novelty detection an automatic emergent property of the network.
“This is a leap forward in understanding how the brain does such a good job of detecting novelty,” said Yuste, noting that the model that Shymkiv created builds on the ideas of John Hopfield, who won the Nobel Prize last year for building neural network models and pioneering artificial intelligence.
The research also offers new insight on the primary role that the cerebral cortex plays in schizophrenia. Clinicians have known for many years that people with schizophrenia are not adept at distinguishing new information from old information. Scientists tried to account for those findings by interpreting the behavior of individual neurons but ended up running into difficulties. One of this paper’s primary insights is its discovery that novelty detection isn’t the work of single neurons but of neural networks.
“We’re very excited that these findings can deepen our understanding of this crucial part of the brain and also potentially offer important insight into cases where those functions go wrong—and ways to fix it,” Yuste said.
Reference: Shymkiv Y, Hamm JP, Escola S, Yuste R. Slow cortical dynamics generate context processing and novelty detection. Neuron. 2025. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2025.01.011
This article has been republished from the following materials. Note: material may have been edited for length and content. For further information, please contact the cited source. Our press release publishing policy can be accessed here.