We've updated our Privacy Policy to make it clearer how we use your personal data. We use cookies to provide you with a better experience. You can read our Cookie Policy here.

Advertisement

Study uncovers details of information processing in the brain

Listen with
Speechify
0:00
Register for free to listen to this article
Thank you. Listen to this article using the player above.

Want to listen to this article for FREE?

Complete the form below to unlock access to ALL audio articles.

Read time: 2 minutes

New research shows that, when focused, we process information continuously, rather in waves as previously thought -


You're in a crowded lecture theatre. Around you are a million tiny distractions: someone rustling in their bag; a door opening for latecomers; a phone vibrating or lighting up; another listener having a snack; a pen dropping on the floor. However, you remain focused, concentrating on the speaker, listening and engaging with the talk.


See Also: How the brain ignores distractions


But, how do you do that?


New research shows that when we're paying attention to something, that information is processed in a continuous manner. But when we're trying to ignore something, we perceive and experience information in waves or frames, like scenes in a movie.


Cognitive neuroscientist Kyle Mathewson and Sayeed Kizuk, graduate of the bachelor of science program in honours psychology and current master of science student, both of the University of Alberta (UofA), recently published research explaining the phenomena.


"We are better at prioritizing certain times when we are not attending to that space in the world," explains Mathewson, assistant professor in the Department of Psychology at UofA and Neuroscience and Mental Health institute affiliate. "This research shows that the two processes for attending to space and attending to time interact with one another."


Our brains oscillate at many different frequencies, explains Mathewson, and each frequency has a different role.


Learn More: Faster brain waves make shorter gaps in the visual stream


"This study examined 12 hertz alpha oscillations, a mechanisms used to inhibit, or ignore, a certain stimulus thereby allowing us to focus on a particular time or space that we are experiencing, while ignoring others," says Mathewson.


For example, if there is a repetitive stimulus in the world, such as the sound of someone's voice in a lecture theatre, the alpha waves lock onto the timing of that stimulus, and the brain becomes better at processing things that occur in time with that stimulus. The new findings show, surprisingly, that this happens more in places we are ignoring.


"We are bombarded with so much information and stimulation that we can't possibly process it all at once. Whether it be commuting, engaging in our work, studying for a class, or working out, our brains select the useful information and ignore the rest, so that we can focus on a single or a few items in order to make appropriate responses in the world. This research helps explain how," says Mathewson.


Mathewson is now working on stimulating the brain at alpha frequencies in order to understand how to improve brain function in meaningful ways. For instance, improving one's ability to focus and perform in real-world situations, such as working on a project or riding a bike.


Read Next: Syllables that oscillate in neuronal circuits


"To better understand how the brain and mind works can help us improve performance and attention in our everyday lives, to improve our safety, increase our work productivity, do better at school, and perform better in sports," explains Mathewson. "We're developing and testing novel, portable technologies to make this possible."


Note: Material may have been edited for length and content. For further information, please contact the cited source.


University of Alberta   Original reporting by: Katie Willis


Publication

Kizuk SAD, Mathewson KE. Power and Phase of Alpha Oscillations Reveal an Interaction between Spatial and Temporal Visual Attention.   Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Published Online October 19 2016. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01058