We've updated our Privacy Policy to make it clearer how we use your personal data.

We use cookies to provide you with a better experience. You can read our Cookie Policy here.

Advertisement
Joyful Screams Processed by the Brain Faster than Screams of Fear or Anger
News

Joyful Screams Processed by the Brain Faster than Screams of Fear or Anger

Joyful Screams Processed by the Brain Faster than Screams of Fear or Anger
News

Joyful Screams Processed by the Brain Faster than Screams of Fear or Anger

Read time:
 

Want a FREE PDF version of This News Story?

Complete the form below and we will email you a PDF version of "Joyful Screams Processed by the Brain Faster than Screams of Fear or Anger"

First Name*
Last Name*
Email Address*
Country*
Company Type*
Job Function*
Would you like to receive further email communication from Technology Networks?

Technology Networks Ltd. needs the contact information you provide to us to contact you about our products and services. You may unsubscribe from these communications at any time. For information on how to unsubscribe, as well as our privacy practices and commitment to protecting your privacy, check out our Privacy Policy

Screaming can save lives. Non-human primates and other mammalian species frequently use scream-like calls when embroiled in social conflicts or to signal the presence of predators and other threats. While humans also scream to signal danger or communicate aggression, they scream when experiencing strong emotions such as despair or joy as well. However, past studies on this topic have largely focused on alarming fear screams.

Humans respond to positive screams more quickly and with higher sensitivity


In a new study, a team at the University of Zurich Department of Psychology led by Sascha Frühholz investigated the meaning behind the full spectrum of human scream calls. The results revealed six emotionally distinct types of scream calls indicating pain, anger, fear, pleasure, sadness and joy. “We were surprised by the fact that listeners responded more quickly and accurately, and with a higher neural sensitivity, to non-alarming and positive scream calls than to alarming screams,” says Frühholz.

Cognitive processing of joyful screams is more efficient


The research team carried out four experiments for their study. Twelve participants were asked to vocalize positive and negative screams that might be elicited by various situations. A different group of individuals rated the emotional nature of the screams and classified them into different categories. While participants listened to the screams, their brain activity underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to monitor how they perceived, recognized, processed and categorized the sounds. “The frontal, auditory and limbic brain regions showed much more activity and neural connectivity when hearing non-alarm screams than when processing alarm scream calls,” explains Frühholz.

More complex social environments have reshuffled neurocognitive priorities


It was previously assumed that human and primate cognitive systems were specially tailored for recognizing threat and danger signals in the form of screams. In contrast to primates and other animal species, however, human scream calls seem to have become more diversified over the course of human evolution – something that Frühholz considers to be a big evolutionary leap. “It's highly possible that only humans scream to signal positive emotions like great joy or pleasure. And unlike with alarm calls, positive screams have become increasingly important over time,” he says. Researchers suggest that this may be due to the communicative demands brought about by humans’ increasingly complex social environments.

Reference:

Frühholz S, Dietziker J, Staib M, Trost W. Neurocognitive processing efficiency for discriminating human non-alarm rather than alarm scream calls. PLOS Biology. 2021;19(4):e3000751. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3000751

This article has been republished from the following materials. Note: material may have been edited for length and content. For further information, please contact the cited source.
Advertisement