We've updated our Privacy Policy to make it clearer how we use your personal data. We use cookies to provide you with a better experience. You can read our Cookie Policy here.

Advertisement

Amputees' Brains Retain a Neural Fingerprint of Their Missing Hand

Amputees' Brains Retain a Neural Fingerprint of Their Missing Hand  content piece image
Credit: Australian Paralympic Committee [CC BY-SA 3.0 (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0) or CC BY-SA 3.0 (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0)]
Listen with
Speechify
0:00
Register for free to listen to this article
Thank you. Listen to this article using the player above.

Want to listen to this article for FREE?

Complete the form below to unlock access to ALL audio articles.

Read time: 2 minutes

Researchers have found that the brain stores detailed information of a missing hand decades after amputation, regardless of whether amputees still experience phantom hand sensations.


Their study, published in eLife, revealed detailed hand information in the brains of amputees compared with people who had been born with a missing hand. The research could pave the way for the development of next-generation neuroprosthetics – prosthetic limbs that tap into the brain’s control centre.


The findings build on the team’s previous work where they used an ultra-high-power MRI scanner to look at the brain activity of two people who had lost their left hand through amputation between two and three decades ago. Although there was less brain activity related to the fingers of the left hand, they found that the specific patterns making up the composition of the hand picture in the brain were well matched to those of two-handed people.


“Our previous findings demonstrated the stability of the hand picture in the cortex despite decades of amputation,” explains lead author Daan Wesselink, a PhD student at the University of Oxford and UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience. “However, we didn’t know whether this hand representation in the brain reflects phantom sensations and therefore only persists in those few people who experience vivid sensations.”


To address this question, they used a brain-decoding technique based on the pattern of brain activity in 18 amputees, who lost their hand to amputation on average 18 years ago and experience varying vividness of phantom sensations. The team also looked at whether development of the hand’s neural fingerprints requires some prior experience of having a hand, by studying 13 people who were missing one hand from birth. They asked both groups to ‘move’ the fingers of their missing and intact hands while in an MRI scanner, and compared the results to two-handed participants.


They found that the brain activity of amputees who had the strongest sensations of being able to move each of their phantom fingers retained the clearest information of their missing hand in their brain. But even those who barely experience phantom hand sensations had the same information preserved in their brains, which was surprising because those amputees have no experiences during their daily life that their brain has held on to this information about their former limb.


By contrast, the group born with one hand showed some brain activity during phantom limb movement, but did not have the same neural fingerprint dedicated to their missing hand. This suggests it could be more challenging to design neuroprostheses or perform hand transplants for this group.


“We’ve shown that once the hand ‘picture’ in the brain is formed, it is generally unlikely to change, despite years of amputation and irrespective of the vividness of phantom sensations,” concludes senior author Tamar Makin, Associate Professor and Sir Henry Dale Fellow at the UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience. “Our work suggests that daily life experience could shape the fine-grained aspects of hand representation, but that the large-scale functional organisation of the hand area is fundamentally stable.”

This article has been republished from materials provided by eLife. Note: material may have been edited for length and content. For further information, please contact the cited source.

Reference: Wesselink, D. B., van den Heiligenberg, F. M., Ejaz, N., Dempsey-Jones, H., Cardinali, L., Tarall-Jozwiak, A., … Makin, T. R. (2019). Obtaining and maintaining cortical hand representation as evidenced from acquired and congenital handlessness. ELife, 8, e37227. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.37227